Skeptic: Examining Extraordinary Claims and Promoting Science Skeptic: Examining Extraordinary Claims and Promoting Science

top navigation:

Wednesday, November 17th, 2010 | ISSN 1556-5696

eSkeptic: the email newsletter of the Skeptics Society

Share this eSkeptic with friends online. Subscribe | Donate | Watch Lectures | Shop



alien at computer (by Daniel Loxton and Jim WW Smith)

Shopping at Amazon this Holiday Season?
HELP THE SKEPTICS SOCIETY
AT NO COST TO YOU!

If you’re planning on shopping at Amazon.com this holiday season, simply start your shopping by clicking on the button below, and the Skeptics Society will receive a commission. Your prices for all Amazon products will remain exactly the same, yet you’ll provide essential financial support for the work of the nonprofit Skeptics Society.

Amazon.com

Prefer to shop at Amazon in Canada, the UK or Germany? No problem! Just click one of these links instead: Amazon.ca, Amazon.co.uk, or Amazon.de.

Bookmark the link and use it all year

For those web savvy types who want to bookmark our Amazon link and use it all year, visit our affiliate links page for easy-to-follow instructions. It will only take seconds, but it will help us for years.


Lecture at Caltech this Sunday

The World in 2050 (book cover)
The World in 2050:
Four Forces Shaping
Civilization’s Northern Future

with Laurence Smith
Sunday, November 21, 2010 at 2 pm
Baxter Lecture Hall

IN A STUNNING VISUAL PRESENTATION, UCLA Professor of Geography and Earth and Space Sciences, Dr. Laurence Smith, forecasts what our planet will be like in the year 2050, distilling his 15 months of research traveling the Arctic Rim with cutting-edge research into four global forces: demographic trends, natural resource demand, climate change, and globalization. The result is both good news and bad: Eight nations of the Arctic Rim (including the U.S.) will become increasingly prosperous, powerful, and politically stable, while those closer to the equator will face water shortages, aging populations, and crowded megacities sapped by the rising costs of energy and coastal flooding. The World in 2050 combines the lessons of geography and history with state-of-the-art model projections and analytical data—everything from climate dynamics and resource stocks to age distributions and economic growth projections.

Tickets are first come first served at the door. Seating is limited. $8 for Skeptics Society members and the JPL/Caltech community, $10 for nonmembers. Your admission fee is a donation that pays for our lecture expenses.


photo

The Nature of Existence

Roger Nygard

What if you could travel the world to ask scientists, artists, spiritual leaders, gurus, and everyday people why we exist? Documentary film-maker Roger Nygard (best known for his films about Star Trek fandom, Trekkies 1 and 2) did just that, conducting over 170 interviews to explore The Nature of Existence.

This week on Skepticality, Swoopy talks with Roger about his journey across the globe, and what he discovered during his compelling conversations with people of faith and science (including Richard Dawkins, Leonard Susskind, and Skeptics Society founder Michael Shermer).


About this week’s feature article

In 1998, at eleven years of age, a young girl named Emily Rosa published a study in the Journal of the American Medical Association. Emily had conceived and executed an experiment that challenged the basis of one of the most accepted “alternative” healing procedures known: Therapeutic Touch (TT). She became the youngest person ever to have a research paper published in a peer reviewed medical journal. In this week’s feature article, Larry Sarner (chairman of the National Therapeutic Touch Study Group) discusses Emily Rosa’s investigation.

The original Rosa paper can be found on the JAMA website and the raw data can be found on Quackwatch, a site operated by senior co-author Stephen Barrett.

This article appeared in Skeptic magazine volume 6, number 2 in 1998.

Share this article
with your friends online.


illustration by Pat Linse

The “Emily Event”
Emily Rosa and the Therapeutic Touch Wars

by Larry Sarner

APRIL FOOL’S DAY, 1998, DAWNED CLEAR AND WARM in New York City. Three people, with an ungainly piece of cardboard and a towel, exited a hotel overlooking Central Park and clambered into a limousine marked “Emily.” They picked their way through traffic towards Rockefeller Center and an appearance on NBC’s Today Show, the first of many that day. Despite the weather, the Rosas were in the eye of a whirlwind of activity and controversy which continues only slighted abated today.

It was the first day of an event unprecedented in the contemporary skeptical movement. Eleven-year-old Emily Rosa had conceived and executed an experiment that challenged the basis of one of the most accepted “alternative” healing procedures known: Therapeutic Touch (TT). It was reported in one of the most respected of medical journals — the Journal of the American Medical Association (JAMA). (Preliminary results of the experiment were originally published in Skeptic magazine vol. 4, no. 4 (1996) and vol. 5, no. 2 (1997)

The appearances Emily and her parents made on April 1st were only the beginning. Michael Shermer, publisher of Skeptic, later made a post on the internet to announce “Shermer on 20/20, Emily Everywhere!” That was seemingly true. At latest count, 100 distinct print pieces appeared, including the front pages of the New York Times and Los Angeles Times; Associated Press, UPI, and Reuters syndicated stories (reprinted countless times, often on front pages). Time reported it as “Emily’s Little Experiment” and People magazine’s title, “Touchy Touchy,” revealed the sensitive nerve it had pressed. There was immediate coverage overseas in The Lancet and the British Medical Journal, and numerous op-ed pieces and letters to the editor appeared everywhere. Television coverage came on all of the evening network news broadcasts, the morning shows, special reports on Fox, CNN, and MSNBC, local coverage, and talk show appearances in Canada and Hollywood, not to mention Scientific American Frontiers coverage the previous November. National Public Radio’s All Things Considered made the initial break of the story, and at least eight talk and news interviews/stories followed on local and syndicated radio, including a blurb on Paul Harvey’s commentary. Out in cyberspace, at least 12 web sites have been discovered dealing with the experiment, and Usenet newsgroups logged over 500 postings, along with several lively discussions on privately run “lists.” So ubiquitous was the coverage that one exasperated TT supporter opined in cyberspace that what he termed the “Emily Event” was necessarily an orchestration by an evil conspiracy of Big Media, Big Science, Big Medicine, and Big Pharmaceuticals.

Conspiracy theories aside, was the Emily Event all media hype, or did it have a wider and enduring importance? One hopes it has the latter, but only time will tell. Despite the uncertainty of the outcome, the nature of its importance is possible to outline. First, it shows a way for scientific medicine and nursing to deal with pseudoscientific proposals for practice, such as TT. Second, it vividly demonstrates the effectiveness of capturing the public imagination for a cause, even if that cause is something as fundamentally cerebral as skepticism and critical thinking.

Nurse Heal Thyself

For some time, scientifically-based medicine has been on the defensive from an onslaught of pseudoscientific, non-scientific, and even anti-scientific practices. The proponents of “alternative,” “complementary,” and now “integrative” medicine have been prying for years at openings in the scientific “establishment.” Having found a few weaknesses — not the least of which is political — their persistence is paying off. Emboldened by a lack of effective sanctions against them, many people whose reputations would have been destroyed by being labeled as charlatans and quacks not only openly promote their “alternatives,” but also feel free to attack their scientific detractors as cranks and reactionaries. The growth of anti-reason and anti-science in medicine is as much the consequence of a lack of response by scientific medicine as it is of anything else.

The battle is not lost, however. Part of the immensity of the response to Emily’s “Little Experiment” is due to a visceral acceptance on the part of the American public of the validity of reason and science. Put in its starkest terms, the public recognized through this event:

  1. that a patently silly practice had become widespread in an important segment of health care;
  2. that it could have been unmasked simply, but wasn’t for 25 years; and
  3. that it was literally child’s play to do that unmasking.

This “Emperor’s New Clothes” story was not lost on the media when they discovered it. In fairness, it was not lost on the medical establishment, either, who published it, setting off the event. But as dramatic as it was, the Emperor aspect should not obscure the way in which the unmasking was done — by reporting good science (as JAMA editor George Lundberg characterized it), albeit long overdue. In other words, the Emily Event happened because scientific medicine finally did what it is supposed to do — science.

How Emily Did It

TT practitioners reacted to Emily’s achievement with a visceral response. Most were ill-informed or logically fallacious (see below). Unable to refute the science itself, the attacks have now settled upon a single theme: that the Rosa paper (and JAMA) went too far in using a single experiment to “disprove” TT. That theme gets to the heart of the medical importance of the Emily Event.

The original experiment did not set out to debunk TT, nor did the paper claim it. In actuality, however, TT was debunked — not as a refutation of the Human Energy Field concept (as erroneously claimed by Lundberg in a note accompanying an abstract of the article). Instead it discounted TT as a healing modality by mounting an empirical assault on the alleged basis for TT practice, i.e., on its five “postulates,” or rather on one of them.

TT practice, as a “unique and efficacious” modality (the justification for practice), rests on its postulates in the following manner: The hands can be used to manifest a TT practitioner’s intention to heal by detecting and eliminating differences in the HEF of a patient, thereby either returning the patient directly to health or removing impediments to the healing processes of the patient’s own body. It can be readily seen that if any one of the postulates can be shown to be logically invalid or to contradict reality, then the TT hypothesis falls. The truth or reality of the others would then be irrelevant. Thus, if the HEF can be shown not to exist, contradicting the Existence Postulate (see point 1 in sidebar above), then TT practice would necessarily be shown as not a unique intervention. If the hands can be shown to be an unreliable means of effecting intentional changes to the health of a patient, contradicting the Manual Postulate (see point 5 in sidebar above), then the TT hypothesis would necessarily fall. And so on.

Most of TT’s skeptics have in the past focused on the Existence Postulate, probably because it has the least ring of truth to it. Its lack of objective detectability especially seems to beg for debunking. However unfortunate, disproof of the existence of the HEF has been intractable, and the Rosa paper did not attempt it. Most TT proponents have in the past focused on the Manual Postulate. They cite dozens of clinical studies which purport to show TT “works.”

They have had the wrong handle on the logical problem. Even if the clinical studies demonstrated unequivocally the reliability and efficacy of TT’s hand-waving (which they do not), it still says nothing about the truth of the other postulates. Because Emily’s experiment utilized practitioners’ hands, it appears that it was a test of the Manual Postulate. That has led many of TT’s defenders to attack the paper for ignoring the “numerous” clinical studies which would appear to contradict Emily’s results. It has also led some TT skeptics to attack the experiment for not being double-blinded (a necessary feature in clinical studies). Regardless of appearances, however, Emily’s little experiment was not a test of the Manual Postulate. The Rosa paper reviewed the clinical studies extensively (and their mixed results). Logically, it did not need to do so, however. Those studies — and even double-blinding — were irrelevant to the logical argument being made in the paper.

Emily Experiment Diagram

click image to enlarge

Emily’s experiment is simple enough for anyone to try. The TT practitioner is shielded from visual contact with the experimenter by a cardboard barrier. A towel prevents peeking through the armholes. A coin is tossed to determine which one of the practitioner’s hands the experimenter’s hand will be hovered over. The practitioner states which hand detects the experimenter’s human energy field (HEF).

What the Emily experiment was in fact was a test of the Perceptibility Postulate (III). It was a refutation of the notion that practitioners can perceive an HEF under conditions where they should have been able to do so. The power of Emily’s experiment was that chances were vanishingly small that the Perceptibility Postulate could be true if the experiment turned out as it did. That observational result constitutes a “refutation,” which leads inescapably to the inference that the TT hypothesis falls. And that in turn leads to the conclusion, explicitly made in the Rosa paper by its senior author, Stephen Barrett, MD that “the further professional use of Therapeutic Touch is unjustified.” It is that reasonable conclusion which apparently was compelling enough for the editorial board of the Journal of the American Medical Association, after an especially prolonged and intensive peer-review, to publish the study, despite its origin in a child’s science-fair project.

That origin, of course, is what captured the widespread attention of the media and medicine-watchers world-wide. And it is the magnitude of that attention which particularly disquiets TT proponents and alternative-medicine advocates, for it brings to light not only the good science of Emily’s experiment, but also the contrast which it makes with the paucity of fundamental and unequivocal evidence for TT.

That leads to the lesson that medicine should take away from the Emily Event: don’t ignore this stuff. The only way for quackery to succeed is for good scientists to do nothing. Don’t just attack it — refute it. True, it’s a Herculean task (there are an infinity of ways for people to be wrong), and not everyone is suited to do it, but someone must.

The medical importance of the Emily Event rests with the change that may come about as a result. The most obvious and measurable change will be in the teaching and practice of TT. Noting the public visceral reaction to the scientific disproof of TT should cause deans of nursing to take a second look at their curricula and weed out TT or analogous kinds of programs. At the very least, TT should be relegated to the “research” back rooms of nursing academia, which it never should have left in the first place.

The early results outside of academia are mixed. Quite surprisingly, a holistic clinic in Rhode Island quietly dropped TT from its offerings because of the results of Emily’s experiment. Yet there are other, mainstream hospitals, like the medical center in Kansas that announced that “after careful consideration” it would continue to offer TT, citing theoretical justification found in quantum physics and Florence Nightingale’s “spiritual philosophy.”

The Top 10 Objections to the Rosa Paper … with Retorts

Since the Rosa paper poses a direct threat to many with vested interests in TT, those interests have advanced specious arguments to discredit the paper and its conclusions. Below is a catalog of the top 10 objections made by TT apologists, with responses.

10 Emily’s experiment was worthless because it wasn’t doubleblinded. The authors are biased against TT and any experimental results reported by them must be discounted. Emily’s parents put her up to this so they could destroy TT.

Just how was this bias supposed to have its effect on the experimental outcomes? Yes, it is important to control for “experimenter bias,” but not foolishly so. You don’t do it just to be doing it, there must be a reason for doing so. Critics have yet to come up with any valid reasons for double-blinding, and some of the reasons put forward by critics are more bizarre than what was being tested. (For example, that Emily could stop the spinning of a coin by holding her hand several inches above it.) Double-blinding is compellingly important in clinical tests, but much less so in other types of experimentation. Emily’s controls were sufficient for the experimental situation. It was a simple binary test. Either it was the right hand or the left. Either the subject said right or left. There were no shades of meaning or correctness to influence.

As for Emily’s parents putting her up to this, Emily herself bristles at this as it takes away credit from her she rightfully believes is hers. Her parents acted as teachers before and during the test, and helped her analyze and understand the results afterwards. They have raised her in an environment that puts a premium on the efficacy of the scientific method (as opposed to “other ways of knowing”), and they make no apologies for that. Emily’s mother is an RN with a professional (and legal) responsibility to report substandard practice to responsible authorities within her profession. This responsibility extended to the results of Emily’s test: it would have been professionally irresponsible for her to suppress or fail to report it.

9 The statistics were flawed. In particular, the number of subjects was too few to warrant the conclusions reached by the authors. Practitioners were expected to get 100% right, which is unreasonable.

The study did no such thing. Logic “expected” practitioners to get 100% right since they claim to be able to manipulate consistently human energy fields for patient benefit, but the statistics analyzed the results only on the basis of getting 67% or 75% right. Yet subjects failed to do better than what would be expected from mere guessing. Indeed, the original protocol declared that a practitioner would have “passed” the test if she got 8 out of 10. Only one did that well, and in a retest got 6 out of 10.

Emily had more practitioners in her study than the vast majority of previous TT studies had patients for theirs. The study’s statistics take into account the size of the sample. Thus, as reported in the paper, if TT practitioners had the ability to feel an energy field with their hands at least two out of every three times they try, the probability of missing it with Emily’s test (even with only 21 practitioners) was less than 1 out of 20. If they had the ability to detect the field at least 3 times out of 4, then the chance of missing it with Emily’s test was only 3 out 10,000.

8 The practitioners were not validly screened as being truly “representative” of TT. In fact, one even reported being a practitioner for longer than TT has been around.

As to being unrepresentative, in what way? All claimed an ability beforehand, all reported at least one year of experience with TT, and one had even published on TT and appeared in a teaching video. There is no “certification” process for TT practitioners, so it is not possible in testing to do anything but take their statements about their abilities at face value. In fact that was the reason why the allegedly anomalous “length of practice” was reported for one practitioner — it was the number of years stated by the practitioner herself. Assuming honesty on her part, it was likely she was reporting that she had been performing something like TT for that length of time. One point is clear after Emily’s test: from now on, reference to a “qualified TT practitioner” must be to one who can demonstrably and objectively detect a patient’s human energy field. We know of no one so qualified.

7 Emily’s field was probably all wrong for this test. She either had (a) cold or bioenergetically dead hands; (b) a wild pubescent field; (c) too perfect a field (or too healthy); (d) a field with too short a range; (e) a field so large it encompassed both hands of a subject; or (f) suppressed or misdirected her field to fool her subjects (either consciously or unconsciously).

In the second go-round, subjects were allowed to first “feel” the energy field around their hands and pick a hand for her to use in the subsequent test. If they couldn’t feel anything at all at that point, why didn’t they say something? Nothing in the previous literature on TT suggests significant obstacles or limitations to the practice of TT. With this notion, the “TT Self-Assessment” procedure (where nurses familiarize themselves by feeling the energy field between their own hands) could never work. Also, in TT originator Dolores Krieger’s book she talks about her own “Emperor’s Clothes” test where you can tell the difference when a shoulder is covered with plastic and when it is not; no warning is given that this test is invalid if the shoulder is healthy (not to mention the state of the practitioner). Also, Krieger claims to feel the energy fields of apparently normal seeds, rivers, rocks, etc. (and to tell the difference between them).

As for redirecting her energy, it should be remembered that Emily was performing the role of a patient in this experiment. It has long been maintained that the beliefs and attitudes of patients (i.e., the people whose fields practitioners feel) have no bearing on the effectiveness of TT practice. This argument also belies the need for double-blinding. Regardless of who was holding out the “target” hand, this argument could always be used to call the target into question. With such obviously contradictory explanations or rationalizations, the point is made for the necessity for fundamental testing, such as Emily’s. It should be noted that each of these reasons, if accepted, would lead to a fundamental untestability of the HEF premise. A failure could always be explained away.

6 This was not a test of TT, but a parlor game. What the practitioners were required to do during the experiment invalidated its applicability to TT, especially since TT is a holistic process and can’t be validly analyzed in parts.

Emily’s test was not of efficacy or technique (or “healing”), but of raw ability. It’s very much like testing a surgeon to see if he can tell, without looking, in which hand the scalpel is being held. In any event, there was some movement. Emily presented her hand after each coin flip, which required relative movement between her hands and the subject’s. Both subjects and Emily had at least small movements of their hands during the trials, and some practitioners even wiggled their fingers or hands. Previous descriptions of the sensations of feeling an HEF state that the field itself is constantly in motion, and the literature states that such motion can be easily felt. Significantly, all of Emily’s subjects agreed to the protocol and none voiced any concern that the test setup would pose a problem in demonstrating their ability.

The argument about TT being “holistic” is a thinly disguised attempt to get back to “outcome” (i.e., clinical) testing, where it is easier to obfuscate, ignore negative results, or explain away nonconforming data. There have been numerous clinical trials on outcomes using TT. The results are highly mixed. Some tests do not have statistically significant results, others revealed slight positive effects (though statistically significant), and several actually reported statistically significant effects, but negative (i.e., the control group did better than the TT group). Holistic practitioners’ prejudice against what they call “reductionism” (analyzing things in parts) is not shared by others in scientific medicine.

5 The conclusions reached by the authors were illogical and outrageous. Nobody ever said that feeling a human energy field was necessary to practice TT, and in fact one doesn’t need to.

TT theorists have said so — consistently. Some “energy workers” now say they do not have to feel the field to be effective, but for 25 years TT practitioners undeniably have claimed to feel the HEF and to assess and manipulate it. It is the raison d’être of TT (as applied energy work). A quarter-century of conducting clinical studies and book writing by TT advocates makes it clear that (until this experiment came along) detecting the energy field was an indispensable part of TT practice.

4 It doesn’t matter how TT works. Even if it’s only a placebo, what’s the harm? TT is too valuable not to be used.

How do they know it works or is truly valuable? By anecdotal evidence and “other ways of knowing” (e.g., wishful thinking, whim, insights through meditation, “grounded theory”, magic, etc.)? Sorry, science requires much more. But for the sake of argument, suppose that TT works only as placebo. How could it be ethically practiced non-experimentally on real patients? The practitioner needs to deceive the patient as to the efficacy and the mechanism of treatment (knowing full well it is only placebo). Deceit is never ethical, even with good intentions. Perhaps TT practitioners and others who argue this point are believers in the-ends-justify-the-means. Fortunately for the rest of us, responsible medical practitioners believe otherwise. Science requires much more.

3 Publication of this was a travesty. It was an insult to qualified, hard-working, grownup nurse researchers.

Why? Because it was done by a kid? What is the minimum age for thinking scientifically? A panel of 15 JAMA editors passed this on for publication. They were highly mindful that if this particular study didn’t pass muster, the journal’s reputation would suffer. Had they suppressed it for non-scientific reasons, they would have been guilty of ignoring facts. JAMA editor George Lundberg put it well: “Age doesn’t matter. All we care about is good science. This was good science.”

Some of these “qualified, hard-working grownups” are not without reason for feeling threatened. Publication of Emily’s experiment is embarrassing to those with Ph.D.’s or DNS’s doing “research” in this area, many of whom actually owe their degrees to TT. It is also embarrassing to all of nursing for letting them get away with it for so long. If their research was so good, so undeniable, so revolutionary, why weren’t their theories recognized by other scientific fields long before Emily was even born?

2 JAMA published this because simple, inexpensive remedies like TT threaten the economic interests of doctors and pharmaceutical companies. They won’t publish research on things that will save consumers money.

In truth, most papers on alternative medicine received by major (mainstream) journals are hopelessly poor in quality. Reading those papers that are published in the lesser journals confirms this. And, of course, there is the question of whether there is anything scientifically valid to report favorably about any alternative practice.

To accept the conspiratorial premise on which this argument is based, one needs to accept also that the conspiracy is not only vast, but is ubiquitous. One has to accept that there is absolutely no one of integrity at the top of the medical professions. And one must accept also that the practicing professionals are so completely impotent, ignorant, or uncaring that they are all unable even to detect the conspiracy, much less fight it to get what is right for their patients.

1 TT works, Emily or no Emily. Let’s talk about something else.

And supposedly open minds slam shut. Next time the statement is made that “skeptics are closed-minded,” remember the Emily Event and consider whose minds were actually closed to evidence.

Skeptic Know Thyself

Skeptics have a chance to see if immense and favorable public opinion can achieve the modest goal of rooting out of nursing an obvious and scientifically disproved wackiness. If not, then perhaps it can be fairly said that skepticism is a toothless watchdog. But if change does come, then the cause of rational thinking and the tactic of exposing nonsense to public scrutiny are vindicated and a pattern is set for tackling future targets. Skeptics now need to mobilize and press home the advantage on TT.


Skeptical perspectives on pseudoscience
cover Trick or Treatment: The Undeniable Facts About Alternative Medicine
(hardback $25.95) by Simon Singh & Edzard Ernst, MD

An excellent guide to the confusions and contradictions of alternative medicine written with clarity, integrity and authority. What works? Who can you trust? What alternative cures have positive results? What medical authorities are included in their “Top ten culprits in the promotion of unproven and disproven medicine?” READ more and order the book.

cover Skeptic magazine vol. 7, no. 4: Pseudoscience
(back issues $6)

Thought Field Therapy; Nazi Medicine; Caroline Myss and Holistic Medicine Evaluated; The Faustian Bargain; Entwined Lives: Twins and What They Tell Us about Human Behavior… READ more and order the back issue.
SUBSCRIBE to Skeptic magazine.

cover The Skeptic Encyclopedia
(two hardback volumes for $129) edited by Michael Shermer

Acupuncture, alien abductions, astrology, facilitated communication, Feng Shui, hypnosis, magnetic therapy, polygraph and lie detection, prayer and healing, recovered memories, reincarnation, spiritualism, subliminal perception, Tut’s curse, UFOs and dozens more… READ more and order the books.


Follow Michael Shermer on Twitter, Facebook, and Skepticblog
Scientific American cover

NEW ON MICHAELSHERMER.COM
The Skeptic’s Skeptic

In Michael Shermer’s November Scientific American column, he espouses the value of clear logic and rhetorical style (layered on top of the usual dollop of data) in communicating scientific ideas.

READ THE POST

Cool it (website detail)

NEW ON SKEPTICBLOG.ORG
Throwing Cold Water
on a Hot Topic

In this week’s Skepticblog post, Michael Shermer reviews Cool It, a film by Bjorn Lomborg, directed by Ondi Timoner, produced by Roadside Attractions and 1019 Entertainment.

READ THE POST

FOLLOW MICHAEL SHERMER ON TWITTER Facebook SKEPTICBLOG
22 Comments »

22 Comments

  1. Thomas Goodey says:

    > Dr. Laurence Smith, forecasts
    > what our planet will be like in the
    > year 2050…The result is both good news
    > and bad: Eight
    > nations of the Arctic Rim (including
    > the U.S.) will become
    > increasingly prosperous,
    > powerful, and politically stable,
    > while those closer to the equator
    > will face water shortages,
    > aging populations, and crowded
    > megacities sapped by the rising
    > costs of energy and coastal flooding.

    And you are suggesting to us that we shouldn’t be skeptical about this risible attempt to predict conditions in forty years from now, through the mirror of global warming propaganda and feelgood/badism?

    The Skeptics Society should not be lending itself to such nonsense.

    • Jeff says:

      So there are still climate change ‘skeptics’ out there, eh Thomas? What impenetrable rock have you been hiding under lately, or are you just another teabugger who thinks reality doesn’t apply to him?

      Of course predictions about the future can never be precise, but the one thing we do know is that climate change is already happening, and faster than anyone thought it would. If nothing is done the change will become even more rapid, far outpacing the ability of life on earth to adapt and leading to truly disastrous consequences.

      But you have your opinion, dont’cha?

      • Another Point of View says:

        You say climate change is a fact, that is real news.Climate change has always been a fact. What is the real issue is what the effect will be and whether it is a good or bad thing. Unless you claim that it will eliminate all life on earth in the near future then how do you make the judgment whether the change will be good or bad. Will mankind be eliminated or will it be the impetus for mankind to adapt and perhaps become stronger and smarter. Will species that disappear be replaced by species that are more adaptive. Will the area on the planet that becomes less habitable eventually renew itself and after some future climate change become more productive than it is now. Will the areas that are not now habitable become places that are more so and more productive.
        To dinosaurs, climate change was a disaster but it was not a disaster for the world. As a human being, I hope that we and our descendants for very many generations do not cease to exist, but they will cease to exist as we know ourselves within a relatively short time, a few thousand years at least. People have changed tremendously in the last 2000 years, that is how we become better (my evaluation). We learn from experimentation, both positively and negatively.

  2. George Campbell says:

    The “Emily Event” was poorly written. Although I read the entire article I still don’t know waht Emily did and what the event was. Obviously it refuted TT, but what was the event?

    • Dr. Latero Sidethink, Hp.D ( RJ Pease)j says:

      The diagram gives a nice picture describing HOW the experiment was done put particulars are sadly lacking in this article.
      the article was remiss in failure to include this reference:

      L Rosa, E Rosa, L Sarner, S Barrett, “A Close Look at Therapeutic Touch,” Journal of the American Medical Association, April 1, 1998; 279(13):1005-1010. [1]

      The details are described in the wiki article on Emily Rosa

      Dr. S

  3. A.K. says:

    George, the experiment is explained below the diagram of the experiment, which is to the right within the body of the article.

  4. Jeff says:

    I was ecstatic when Emily’s paper was published, thinking that finally something would be done about the ridiculous fraud called Therapeutic Touch – since renamed to Healing Touch in a lame attempt at spin-doctoring.

    Well, it’s still going strong, in fact the “nurse of the year” at one of our local hospitals is a practitioner of this and other ‘holistic’ bullshit. And homeopathic ‘remedies’ are still being sold all over the place, thanks to an apathetic Congress that responds only to the loudest voices and deepest pockets rather than protecting the public from irresponsible frauds.

    It seems to me that our society is running backwards as fast as it can, longing for the “good old days” which were anything but. The future is going to be an awfully bumpy ride, and I’m thankful I won’t have to live through most of it at my age. Good luck, kiddies.

  5. Tony Q. King says:

    Thanx for the intro on the re-branding crap- I’ve seen it go from “alternative” to “complementary”, but was not aware of the latest moniker- “integrative”.
    They still have a big shelf of homoeopathic remedies at my local pharmacy (in Quebec, Canada)- perhaps I should inform them of the new-improved name. ;)

  6. Richard Prins says:

    I remember Emily Rosa from John Stossel’s The Power of Belief. A poor quality copy of that segment is on YouTube @ http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=H5HdznWGi7o

  7. Nick McRoberts says:

    I read with interest the Emily Rosa article. While the refutation of postulate 3 is elegant, the method used is open to attack as outlined briefly (but not exhaustively) below, AND does not “logically” refute the therapeutic usefulness of TT, only the claimed pseudo-scientific pseudo-medical mechanism.

    The Rosa study is single-blind. While compelling, a better experiment would be double-blind. 3 serious objections to the method can and should be considered which make the study incomplete but not irrelevant.
    1) Study author selection bias. The method described for the selection of study subjects is open to criticism as it allows for significant author bias (authors chose participants form advertising and “other” leads).
    2) The sample size is small : 21 out of (conservative sample size) 43000. The TT practitioner participants do not represent statistically the TT practitioner sample (only one US state and a very small sample). This does not prove that the study is irrelevant – just that it only deals with a small number of selected practitioners.
    2) Experimental conditions. A better (more rigorous) experiment would necessarily include a control group (non TT partitioners) and a researcher who did not know the difference between the practitioners and the control group.

    The more serious flaw in this article is the fallacious reasoning that leads the author to disregard the clinical usefulness of TT based on its obviously non-existant biological mechanical properties. As pointed out by the author, the method appears to rely entirely on the placebo effect to obtain results. A number of important and interesting studies show that the placebo effect is a powerful and little-understood tool for healing. A study comparing TT with other placebo-type remedies for their actual usefulness would give better indications as to whether the practice has any useful (if completely unscientific) by-products that can be used in medicine.

  8. R J Reynolds says:

    I have just struggled through the (almost) complete Emily discussion and I am convinced that the story, in every significant detail, could be condensed to less than one third of the original without loss of anything significant (except show-offy verbosity). As it stands, it is a monster that needs taming. I bet there is not one reader who finishes it word for word.

    PLEASE be brief(er).

    R J

  9. JRW says:

    I could never trust the medical industry to have my best interests in mind as long as they have financial interest in my treatment. I have any number of instances where the medical industry has exploited another person’s misfortune for their own profit.

  10. RJ Pease says:

    Another reason why the experiment is invalid…

    Since this works by Magick energy, it is a widely
    observed phenomenon that Magick Energy is diluted by the presence of Sceptical energy in the vicinity.

    See Randi’s “Flim Flam” for more details .

    Dr. Sidethink Hp.D

  11. Andy O says:

    The only valid objection to this study would be that if the study had FOUND practitioners who could consistently detect Emily’s hand, it would NOT have been submitted for publication. We’ll never know the outcome of that.

  12. Jeff Swift says:

    What a waist of time and energy. To believe or disbelieve will always be a matter of choice, no amount of experimentation or demonstration will ever change that.
    Scientists are motivated by their desire to dominate others, just like everyone else. Even a 4 year old can say “I am right and you are wrong” and she might be smart enough to prove it at 11, next subject please. Hopefully she will gain some maturity as she approaches adulthood.

    • Joe Hilbig says:

      Bullshit. Shoveled by someone willfully ignorant of how science works.

      Scientists are (mostly) motivated by a desire to know things.

      Ignorant people insist that their uninformed beliefs trump results obtained through education, intelligence and lots of work.

  13. Udo says:

    I would be surprised of some of the Practitioners could not detect the heat radiation from the Experimenter’s hand; as shown in the illustration they are quite close. An experiment that would quell most of the objections of the quacks and skeptical skeptics would require some method of double blinding and substitution of a heated, body temperature, non-organic hand for some percentage of the trials.

    But then why bother, is it not the burden of the woo believers to provide evidence when a practice is not supported by any rigorous studies? Who would waste time and resources on an improved study when real science that benefits humanity could be undertaken as opposed to discrediting those that will not accept the results anyway?

  14. Seorsa says:

    I agree with the criticisms of the flawed logic that rejects the significance of the placebo effect. The fact that the standard for new medications is to be more effective than the placebo is a tacit acknowledgement of the power of the placebo. I recall a study that had the subjects take a placebo after being told it was s sugar pill and the placebo effect was not any less powerful. The conclusion that the technique does not have any usefulness for healing would seem to require a clinical experiment. Many new drugs are rejected for not being better than the placebo in clinical trials. This does not mean that they had no effect, or had no positive outcomes. Since it seems irrefutable that the placebo effect is real, the skeptic who has an interest in clinical outcomes should have a duty to ask the question “Does TT produce a sufficient placebo effect to be useful in treatment? and treatment of what?

  15. ThePenguin says:

    Am I the only one who has noticed that the story ‘broke’ on 1 April? April Fool?

  16. Kirsten Abbott says:

    Why such a burning desire to ban TT? At least, it doesn’t hurt anybody. There are people who make an educated choice to forgo the blessings of modern medicine as much as they can. There are people who just like TT. So, why ban it? Advertise “scientific” remedies and let people choose. That’s the emphasis – educate people and let them choose. Don’t try to use governmental agencies to jam modern medicine down people’s throats without allowing them to have any alternative. And before launching a crusade on anything, remember how many people DIE every year due to the side-effects of “scientific” drugs and mistakes in the modern diagnostics – the statistics are available.

get eSkeptic
our free newsletter

Science in your inbox every Wednesday!

eSkeptic is our free email newsletter, delivered once a week. In it, you’ll receive: fascinating articles, announcements, podcasts, book reviews, and more…


Popular Articles
on skeptic.com

Here are the articles that people have been sharing over the last few days.

Carbon Comic

Carbon Comic (by Kyle Sanders)

Carbon Comic, which appears in Skeptic magazine, is created by Kyle Sanders: a pilot and founder of Little Rock, Arkansas’ Skeptics in The Pub. He is also a cartoonist who authors Carbon Dating: a skeptical comic strip about science, pseudoscience, and relationships. It can be found at carboncomic.com.

Help the
Skeptics Society
at no cost to you!

Planning on shopping at Amazon? By clicking on our Amazon affiliate link, which will open the Amazon Store in your Internet browser, the Skeptics Society will receive a small commission on your purchase. Your prices for all products remain the same, yet you’ll provide essential financial support for the work of the nonprofit Skeptics Society.

amazon.com

See our affiliate links page for Amazon.ca, Amazon.de, Amazon.co.uk, iTunes, and Barnes & Noble links.

FREE PDF Download

The Science Behind Why People See Ghosts

The Science Behind Why People See Ghosts

Do you know someone who has had a mind altering experience? If so, you know how compelling they can be. They are one of the foundations of widespread belief in the paranormal. But as skeptics are well aware, accepting them as reality can be dangerous…

Reality Check

Reality Check: How Science Deniers Threaten Our Future (paperback cover)

How Science Deniers Threaten Our Future

The battles over evolution, climate change, childhood vaccinations, and the causes of AIDS, alternative medicine, oil shortages, population growth, and the place of science in our country—all are reaching a fevered pitch. Many people and institutions have exerted enormous efforts to misrepresent or flatly deny demonstrable scientific reality to protect their nonscientific ideology, their power, or their bottom line…

FREE PDF Download

Top 10 Myths About Evolution

Top 10 Myths About Evolution (and how we know it really happened)

If humans came from apes, why aren’t apes evolving into humans? Find out in this pamphlet!

FREE PDF Download

Top 10 Things You Should Know About Alternative Medicine

Top 10 Things You Should Know About Alternative Medicine

Topics include: chiropractic, the placebo effect, homeopathy, acupuncture, and the questionable benefits of organic food, detoxification, and ‘natural’ remedies.

FREE PDF Download

Learn to be a Psychic in 10 Easy Lessons

Learn to do Psychic “Cold Reading” in 10
Easy Lessons

Psychic readings and fortunetelling are an ancient art — a combination of acting and psychological manipulation.

Copyright © 1992–2014 Skeptic and its contributors. For general enquiries regarding the Skeptics Society or Skeptic magazine, email skepticssociety@skeptic.com or call 1-626-794-3119. Website-related matters: webmaster@skeptic.com. Enquiries about online store orders: orders@skeptic.com. To update your subscription address: subscriptions@skeptic.com. See our Contact Information page for more details. This website uses Google Analytics, Google AdWords, and AddThis tracking software.